Technique for mentoring pre-readers and early readers

Education and demonstration – Language – Spelling – phonics – word recognition – or sentence formation

Reexamination Certificate

Rate now

  [ 0.00 ] – not rated yet Voters 0   Comments 0

Details

C434S167000, C434S180000, C434S185000

Reexamination Certificate

active

06632094

ABSTRACT:

FIELD OF THE INVENTION
The acquisition of reading proficiency is a cumulative process that requires the guidance of a mentor, commonly a parent or teacher, and practice. The present invention is an audio/visual technique that simulates the guidance of a mentor by presenting customized audio narration together with a video display of printed text having portions thereof highlighted in synchronism with the narration. It is designed to motivate children to practice reading.
BACKGROUND OF THE INVENTION
Educational research has shown that the rate at which a child's reading skills increases is in direct proportion to the amount and frequency with which the child (1) is read to by a mentor and (2) practices reading under the guidance of the mentor.
(1) The Importance of Reading to a Child
In her chapter entitled, “Growing Successful Readers: Homes, Schools, and Communities,” Victoria Purcell-Gates, Ph.D., states:
“If we want children to learn the span of concepts about print, including directionality, eye-voice match, and so forth, we need to have children involved in reading and writing attempts of actual connected text. If we want children to learn about the phonemic base of written English, we must involve them in reading and writing attempts that focus them on discovering it through “sounding out” and with the help of teachers who help them do this. Many believe that this is best done at first with phonetically regular words and text (Juel & Roper/Schneider, 1985), and lots of opportunities to read such text in order to internalize the regularities and promote automaticity of word recognition and processing. If we want children to learn about the vocabulary, syntax, and decontextualized natures of different forms of written discourse, then we must read aloud to them from text that is too difficult for them to read on their own.”
A variety of reading software and electronic devices have been developed to provide some of the mechanical aspects of reading to a child (e.g., providing the sound of a letter, phoneme, or word on demand and highlighting words while their sounds are played). In their book “Learning to Read in the Computer Age,” Anne Meyer, Ed.D., and David Rose, Ed.D., describe the progress made in the usefulness of computer software to improve reading skills:
“Using a computer, students can write, ask a word or letter to say its own name, have a story read, see the lyrics to a song while listening to it . . . No other technology approaches this kind of versatility.”
However, room for improvement remains to increase the effectiveness of reading software by adding more of the human aspects of reading to a child. Examples of such human aspects are: reading a story with drama and feeling while pointing to the text as it is read and answering questions about the meaning of words in the story not with dictionary definitions, but with explanations that take the context of the story into account.
(2) The Importance of Practice
Educational research has also shown that reading improvement depends upon the amount of meaningful practice, which in turns depends upon the child's interest in learning to read and willingness to practice. In her chapter entitled “The Three-Cueing System,” Marilyn Jager Adams, Ph.D., states:
“As expected, instructional support . . . generally does result in increases in the productivity of [children's] reading. Yet research also has shown that as children's reading experience grows, [their] capabilities tend to grow alongside. That is, to the extent that children do read, they generally learn new words, new meanings, new linguistic structures, and new modes of thought through reading (Stanovich, 1993).The wisdom of the popular dictum that reading is best learned through reading follows directly. So too, however, does the seriousness of its most nettlesome caveat: Where children find reading too difficult, they very often will not do it-or at least not with the sort of engagement that best fosters learning.”
Regarding the various approaches that have been developed to support the acquisition of reading proficiency, room for improvement also remains to increase children's motivation to practice by make the process interesting, fun, and frustration-free.
Reading Readiness
An additional need exists for a reading support system capable of instilling “reading readiness” in prereaders. In her book “Beginning to Read, Thinking and Learning About Print,” Marilyn Jager Adams, Ph.D., states “The ‘reading ready’ child enters school with a substantial base of prereading skills and a wealth of experience with and knowledge about the pleasures and functions of text and about literary language and styles . . . the likelihood that a child will succeed in the first grade depends most of all on how much she or he has learned already about reading before getting there.”
Dr. Purcell-Gates lists the following five dimensions of “reading readiness”: (1) intentionality, (2) written register knowledge, (3) alphabetic principle knowledge, (4) concepts of writing, and (5) concepts about print. Research studies show that preschoolers' mastery of these five dimensions is a predictor of their later success in learning to read. Details on each of these five dimensions is provided below.
(1) Intentionality
Children who understand intentionality know that print “says something.” They do not have to know what it says; they only have to know that it says something. Thus, when a young child tugs on her mother's sleeve, points to a printed sign (e.g., Exit) over the door in the store and asks, “What does it say?” one may conclude that this child knows that print is a linguistically meaningful system.
2) Written Register Knowledge
Children with written register knowledge possess implicit schemas for the syntax, vocabulary, and reference characteristics of written narrative that are distinct from those they hold for oral speech. Knowledge of written storybook register is knowledge of vocabulary and syntax, and of linguistic ways to maintain meaning within text without reference to the physical world via gesture, intonation, or shared background knowledge.
3) Alphabetic Principle Knowledge
When one masters the alphabetic principle, he or she understands that encoding and decoding English words begins at the phoneme/grapheme level. That is, letters generally stand for individual “phonemes” (speech sounds). Whether one can do this accurately (conventionally map the correct letter to the sound) is beside the point at this stage. Understanding the alphabetic principle is knowing that one must do this to read and write English.
(4) Concepts of Writing
Children who understand concepts of writing conceptualize writing as a system. To measure children's understanding of concepts of writing, researchers hand them pencil and paper and asked them to “Write your name and anything else you can.” Those children with a strong grasp of the concept of writing, write words or pseudo-words. Children who score lowest on this task draw pictures, scribbles, lines around the border of the page, etc. Children who understand concepts of writing know the difference between printed text and illustrations and decorations in a book.
(5) Concepts About Print
Children who understand concepts about print understand that marks called “letters” make up “words,” and “words” make up “sentences” that are marked by periods, question marks, and exclamation marks. They know that these marks are written, and read, beginning at the top left corner of the block of print and across horizontally to the top right corner, at which point one returns to the left end of the line underneath the one just finished, and so on. They know that letters are different from numbers and pictures and that their shapes, orientations and identities are stable across time and across contexts.
Progress in these five dimensions can be attained with one-on-one reading sessions for the child by, for example, a family member or a teacher. However, this has obvious inherent limitations involving a

LandOfFree

Say what you really think

Search LandOfFree.com for the USA inventors and patents. Rate them and share your experience with other people.

Rating

Technique for mentoring pre-readers and early readers does not yet have a rating. At this time, there are no reviews or comments for this patent.

If you have personal experience with Technique for mentoring pre-readers and early readers, we encourage you to share that experience with our LandOfFree.com community. Your opinion is very important and Technique for mentoring pre-readers and early readers will most certainly appreciate the feedback.

Rate now

     

Profile ID: LFUS-PAI-O-3162045

  Search
All data on this website is collected from public sources. Our data reflects the most accurate information available at the time of publication.